Great story in the Economist (actually from last week, but I am a little behind in my reading) that makes exactly the point I made. The story is about how many environmental groups are conveniently in the "follow the scientific consensus" camp when that fits their gut instinct but in the "better safe than sorry" camp when that one fits better.
The two examples in the Economist article are GMO crops and global climate change. When it comes to climate change, environmentalists have a strong argument that the scientific consensus has shown that climate change is caused by human activity. So we need to do something about it.
But then when it comes to GMO crops, the scientific consensus is that there are tremendous benefits (e.g. Golden rice filled with beta carotene, doubled crop productivity in arid regions of Africa) and the only negative evidence was recently retracted because the study was poorly done. But what is the response from a large set of European and Asian environmental groups (and some in the U.S. too)? What if the scientific consensus is wrong?
I was just going to mention this as a comment on the previous post. But then I read this. The main conclusion of 60 years of studies? Human judgment on complicated topics really sucks compared to scientific consensus. Our heuristics are great for short term, simple, straightforward cause-effect decisions. But once you get more than a few factors involved or have to make short term sacrifices for long term gains, we really truly suck at it.
My musings about human behavior and how we can design the world around us to better accommodate real human needs.
Sunday, December 22, 2013
Saturday, December 21, 2013
Bentley rocks the future of UX
I was beaming with pride as I read UX Magazine’s Top UXpredictions for 2014. No, they didn’t
ask me for my prediction. But several of
the top predictions from some of the leading thought leaders in UX are exactly
what we focus on in the Bentley MS in HFID and BS in IDCC programs. It is almost like we looked into the future
ten years ago (actually Bill Gribbons should get the credit here) and tailored
the curriculum for today’s future (one of my favorite oxymorons :-)).
Here are the ones I am talking about:
#1. More UX designers
will develop expertise in marketing and business. That is a no brainer for Bentley, we are one
of the only UX-related programs that are housed in a business school. Both our BS and MS programs, of course. In fact, we are not just a business school,
we are a Business University.
#2. More programs will appreciate the importance of customer
experience (which is a wider concept than user experience). Yup. We do that.
Again, both BS and MS programs.
#3. UX will be blended with content strategy. I just finished teaching our undergrad
content strategy course and 90% of my students were in the IDCC program.
Most of the predictions after #3 are too specific to
describe any particular program. Things
like responsive design and mobile first design.
And some are negative predictions so I am glad we don’t reflect those.
Then when you get towards the end:
#15 and #16: Positive Computing and Conscious Experience. These focus on considering more than just
utility and usability in design – also thinking about what will improve the
lives of the users on a broader scale – emotional health, intrinsic motivation,
human connection. We don’t have courses
in these specifically but they are the areas where I have been focusing my research
lately so we are poised to be leaders there too.
Yeah Bentley!!!
Friday, December 20, 2013
Liberty or Safety first?
As anyone following my blog knows, I love investigating
situations where people clearly hold opposing views at the same time and
experience no internal conflict. So I
found the recent ruling by the New Mexico Supreme Court as a good lead in for a
new post.
Think about this general philosophic question: When the government is considering a regulation
that would constrain some kind of freedom, should the burden of proof be on
proponents to prove that there is no harm or opponents to prove that there is
harm? In other words, do you believe in
a “better safe than sorry” standard where we should prevent anything that isn’t
proven safe? Or do you believe in a “liberty
unless proved otherwise” standard” where we should allow everything, in the
name of liberty, unless there is a clear government interest in banning it
(e.g. it is dangerous)?
It is important to think about this in in the abstract
because of the human irrational thought processing that I blog about so
much. Once we focus on a particular
topic, we first think about what we want to be the policy and then rationalize
that it is the proper philosophical stance.
OK, now think about specific examples and see if you are indeed a real human – i.e. a hypocrite (which is our natural state – sorry to break it to you). I know you will say, "but there is evidence for #1, there isn't evidence for #2!!! But I am asking the question in the abstract. What would have been your starting point before the evidence was in?
WARNING – these are touchy subjects. You will definitely have emotional reactions just to asking the questions. That is my point! Emotional reactions trump your philosophic stance. You will rationalize contradicting what you said earlier on at least one of these?
- Do we allow genetically modified foods until there is evidence of some kind of harm? Or do we ban them until there is evidence that they are safe?
- Do we allow new pharmaceuticals on the market based on efficacy trials until someone proves they have long term harms? Or do we require drug makers to do long term studies, holding drugs that could be helping patients, off the market until they are proved safe?
- Do we regulate greenhouse gases just in case we are destroying the world with them, even at the expense of economic growth? Or do we wait until the evidence is clearer?
- Do we allow the NSA to collect telecommunications metadata until we can prove it doesn’t combat terrorism? Or do we prevent it until they can prove it will help?
- Do we allow same sex couples all of the rights and freedoms of heterosexual couples until there is evidence of harm? Or do we hold off until there is clear evidence there is no harm.
In case you were wondering about the earlier reference to the NW court ruling, they found that the state could not ban same sex marriage until they could prove that there was a clear government interest. In other words, liberty first.
Thursday, December 19, 2013
Social Reinforcement on Hotels.com
Graham likes the notification on Hotels.com where it says
something to the effect of “This hotel has been booked 8 times in the last 24
hours.” Why is this relevant? There are two possible purposes. One, if a lot of people are booking it, it
must be a good place. This is social
reinforcement. Second, if a lot of
people are booking it, it might be full soon.
So you better hurry up. This is an
urgency cue.
Both of these are basic survival motivators. We evolved to be sensitive to scarcity
because if there was only a little left of something important (and all
resources could be in scarce supply 200,000 years ago), it was important for
survival to get some - just in case. We
evolved to be sensitive to social cues because being part of the tribe was also
a key to survival. So viscerally, these
techniques are effective. This also
makes them effective as sales techniques, at least in the short term.
I like urgency in games because I am not suspicious of the
ulterior motive when it comes from a salesperson. I see urgency in a sales context as the
equivalent of those infomercials that scream at you “Buy Now!” and they have a
30 second countdown when the deal will end. I wouldn’t buy from those just out
of spite!! So I prefer designs like
Kayak that give you a simple notice of remaining inventory. “3 Tickets left at this price.” It is still urgency, but it seems more honest. The design is also clear because it is within
a list of flights where some have 2 left and some have no notification at all
(more than 5 left I guess).
Social reinforcement can serve two purposes from a sales
context. For one, it is a signal of
quality. If many people are buying something, then you won’t look like a fool
if you get one too. The signal is even
better if it comes from your social network – 8 of your friends have bought
this. When it comes from your friends it
adds a dimension of appropriateness. If
my friends like it, and I have things in common with my friends, then there is
a better chance that I will like it to.
This is a closer match than 8 random other people.
So urgency and social reinforcement cues can be effective,
but only if they are designed right.
Urgency has to be relevant and seen as authentically intended to help
you rather than to pressure you. If a
shopper is just at the beginning of the search process and is not ready to buy,
then urgency is oppressive. This is the fundamental
psychological motivator called “The need for autonomy.” We want autonomy, and we rebel from anything
that seems intended to constrain us. So
urgency cues should be very subtle at early parts of the shopping funnel and
more salient later.
Wednesday, December 18, 2013
Mining for help
I am going to generalize Graham’s fourth UX element a little
bit to make it a better topic for discussion.
He likes the way Gmail scans your email text for the word “attachment”
and if you don’t attach anything it pops up a notification asking if you meant
to. As someone who forgets to attach
files to emails all the time, I can really appreciate the value of this. And
given that I know lots of other people who also do, I think the feature is a
no-brainer on value.
But what about similar features? What else could an email system search for
and warn you about? Here is one that I
think might be valuable. Semantic analysis
systems are getting pretty good at judging blocks of text for their emotional
valence. I think the state of the art
know is that they can tell if something is very or slightly positive, very or
slightly negative, or neutral. They miss
some things like snarky sarcasm or culturally specific metaphors. But overall they are not too bad and getting
better. So what if your Gmail account
notices that you just wrote a critical email to your boss or your mother and
pops up “Are you sure you want to send this?” kind of message?
I am sure many of you are more creative than I am – what other
good search-based notifications can we come up with for Gmail?
Tuesday, December 17, 2013
History, memory, and reconsolidation
A great discussion this morning on On Point with Jay Parini got me thinking about human memory and what I have learned recently about re-consolidation of episodic memory.
The topic of the show was the history, mythology, and religion surrounding our knowledge of who Jesus really was. There is a lot there, so I am just going to focus on one piece. The Gospels were written 20-40 years or so after the events occurred. The recent anniversary of JFK's assassination and the death of Nelson Mandela provide some good modern analogies. Out of everything you heard last month about JFK and last week about Mandela, how much of it was perfectly true, patently false, or totally ambiguous? I think a lot of it falls into that latter category.
Here is where re-consolidation comes in. Every time we recall a memory (for example of JFK's actions during the Cuban Missile Crisis or Mandela's actions to give the black South African's the vote), the memory gets reframed in the context of our current situation. This is not intentional nor is it a good or bad thing, it is just a function of the way the hippocampus and frontal cortex are wired together. They are using this phenomenon to treat PTSD by having soldiers recall painful experiences in an environment where they feel peaceful and safe. The two feelings get mixed together and the memory becomes less painful.
We can do the same thing with details surrounding a memory. Imagine yourself running around the playground when you were in grade school. You are either remembering a photo of yourself at that age or you are imagining what you currently look like only shorter. This second option is reconsolidation.
So what does this have to do with Jesus, JFK, and Mandela? With the cold war over, we can think of JFK's decisions in a much more positive light. With Mandela's subsequent leadership of South Africa, we can do the same thing with his earlier terrorist activities. I saw a lot of this reframing going on in the past months. The same thing most likely happened with all historical figures, including Jesus. Those days were very exciting to say the least. There was chaos in much of the Middle East. The various sects that were splintering off the mainstream Jews were looking for messiahs. In those 20-40 years, a lot happened that would have affected how Jesus the historical figure was seen.
So no matter what you think of his religious identity, everyone who remembered him would have a very different perspective depending on their frame of reference 40 years later. Even if Jesus was divine, the people remembering him were most definitely human and had brains wired for reconsolidation. Their memories could not be anything other than blended, muddled, mixes of actual memories, imagined memories, and current ideas. Contradictions in the Gospels is not evidence of lying, cheating, or exaggerating, just reconsolidation at work. Not much help when trying to decide what to believe.
The topic of the show was the history, mythology, and religion surrounding our knowledge of who Jesus really was. There is a lot there, so I am just going to focus on one piece. The Gospels were written 20-40 years or so after the events occurred. The recent anniversary of JFK's assassination and the death of Nelson Mandela provide some good modern analogies. Out of everything you heard last month about JFK and last week about Mandela, how much of it was perfectly true, patently false, or totally ambiguous? I think a lot of it falls into that latter category.
Here is where re-consolidation comes in. Every time we recall a memory (for example of JFK's actions during the Cuban Missile Crisis or Mandela's actions to give the black South African's the vote), the memory gets reframed in the context of our current situation. This is not intentional nor is it a good or bad thing, it is just a function of the way the hippocampus and frontal cortex are wired together. They are using this phenomenon to treat PTSD by having soldiers recall painful experiences in an environment where they feel peaceful and safe. The two feelings get mixed together and the memory becomes less painful.
We can do the same thing with details surrounding a memory. Imagine yourself running around the playground when you were in grade school. You are either remembering a photo of yourself at that age or you are imagining what you currently look like only shorter. This second option is reconsolidation.
So what does this have to do with Jesus, JFK, and Mandela? With the cold war over, we can think of JFK's decisions in a much more positive light. With Mandela's subsequent leadership of South Africa, we can do the same thing with his earlier terrorist activities. I saw a lot of this reframing going on in the past months. The same thing most likely happened with all historical figures, including Jesus. Those days were very exciting to say the least. There was chaos in much of the Middle East. The various sects that were splintering off the mainstream Jews were looking for messiahs. In those 20-40 years, a lot happened that would have affected how Jesus the historical figure was seen.
So no matter what you think of his religious identity, everyone who remembered him would have a very different perspective depending on their frame of reference 40 years later. Even if Jesus was divine, the people remembering him were most definitely human and had brains wired for reconsolidation. Their memories could not be anything other than blended, muddled, mixes of actual memories, imagined memories, and current ideas. Contradictions in the Gospels is not evidence of lying, cheating, or exaggerating, just reconsolidation at work. Not much help when trying to decide what to believe.
Easy product views on hover
The second two UX elements that Graham points out are fundamentally
the same idea – using the hover function to support effective browsing. He notes that on Lilly Pulitzer (which sells
women’s apparel), when you hover over a product photo (such as a model wearing
a Lilly Pulitzer dress) on a search results page or on a category page it shows
you the rear view. On Land’s End, you
can easily see an item in a variety of colors. Underneath each product photo there is a
palette of colors that the item is available in. When you click on one of them, the item
photo changes to that color without reloading the page.
For a wide variety of shopping use cases, seeing the rear
view of an item and seeing the item in different colors are both very important. Using the hover control makes it easier and
faster. How many times have you looked at a browsing
function, estimated how long it would take for the page to load, multiplied
this by the number of times you would have to use it while browsing the variety
of products you want to consider, and then decided not to bother? To use this function, you would either have
to invest a lot more time than you planned or you would have to consider just a
few items. Not worth it.
I think the logic behind this is pretty generalizable. Any voluntary experience is a constant
effort/benefit evaluation. How much
physical and mental effort will it be and what will I get out of it? So any UX element has to be a net positive on
this equation to be used and valued.
Changing to a hover function from a page load reduces the time and the
frustration levels for the same benefit (additional views of the item). There are some users on one extreme who would
have used either version because they value the additional views very
highly. There are some users on the other
extreme who would not bother with either version because they don’t value the
additional views much at all. But for
those in the middle, we can add to their shopping experience and increase sales
and satisfaction.
The vendor would have to judge the number of users in this
middle category given its demographics and product category and compare that to
the programming cost of the hover function. If you have decent data on these, it is a
clear decision.
We can complicate this by also throwing in a variable like the time it takes for the hover effect to kick in. If it is too long, the user may never realize that the function exists. If they click and get taken to a detailed product page from the gallery page, then it defeats the purpose. But this is a subject for another post.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)